New approach urged for late-talking bilingual babies
Babies who are raised in homes
where two or more languages are spoken may appear to talk later than those
learning just one language, leaving parents puzzled and concerned as to the
reasons why.
Conventional wisdom often suggests that such
children are confused and so they take longer to talk. Or, parents may hear that
any apparent delay is just an illusion because kids are little geniuses who can
learn many languages quickly and easily.
"Both of these views are wrong," US
psychologist Erika
Hoff told the American Association for the Advancement of Science
meeting in Vancouver this weekend.
"It is not the case that hearing two
languages confuses the child and impairs their ability to acquire anything. But
it is also not the case that children can magically acquire two languages as
quickly as one."
Instead, psychologists should take a
different approach to testing young children, one that measures their
proficiency in both languages instead of just one.
When that is done, researchers typically
find that the two tests add up to about the same level of proficiency as would
be seen in a baby who is learning a single language.
"Children who are exposed to two
languages... must hear less of each language than a child who hears only one and
so it takes them longer to get the same amount of experience with each
language," added Hoff, whose research has focused on highly educated bilingual
Spanish-English families in south Florida.
Two kinds of tests have existed for decades
-- the Language Development Survey and the MacArthur Bates Communicative Development
Inventory -- in which parents answer questions about which words their
child knows and how many word combinations the child has at around age two.
Since their inception decades ago, both
paper-and-pencil surveys have been adapted into different languages, with as
many as 20 variations of the LDS and more than 60 of the MacArthur
Bates now out there, researchers said.
Even this low-tech approach has proven
superior to modern methods, said Philip Dale, professor of speech and hearing
sciences at the University of New Mexico, Albuquerque.
"Despite an understandable skepticism you
might have about the ability of parents with limited training and a natural
pride in their child, parent-report can be quite an accurate measure," said
Dale.
Leslie Rescorla, professor of psychology at
Bryn Mawr college in Pennsylvania, who devised the LDS in the 1980s, agreed that
surveys can be very effective in identifying late-talkers by 24-30 months of
age.
In the LDS, parents are given a 310 word
checklist, and are asked to mark which words their child says. Average children
have about 150 words at that age, and late talkers have 25-50.
Rescorla presented research on new versions of the LDS distributed in Greece,
South Korea and the Netherlands, which showed similar results as have been seen
in the United States.
For instance, eight percent of Greek
children surveyed were found to be late-talkers, compared to nine percent of US
children.
Knowing whether a child is a late-talker is
important because it may point to disorders that could be helped with early
intervention, such as autism, hearing loss, or mental impairment, said Nan
Bernstein Ratner, professor of hearing and speech sciences at the University of
Maryland.
"Late-talkers are at high risk for other developmental problems," said
Ratner."If you have children who have problems with language and with reading, we have children who will not succeed in society."
Previous research has shown that as many as
20 percent of all children are late-talkers, but many of them are simply
"late-bloomers" who catch up by age five, added Ratner.
"About four-fifths of children will recover.
The problem is we don't know which ones, so if you don't have a crystal ball it
is much better to catch them at age two and to start tracking them, rather than
to wait and see what happens."
When it comes to bilingual babies, Hoff
urged parents to take advantage of the second-language tests out there, rather
than worry about the child's poor scores in a single survey which is missing the
full extent of the child's knowledge.
"Because these inventories are available in
multiple languages, you can often assess what they know in both languages," she
said.
"When you do that, the bilingually
developing children look exactly like the monolingually developing child. They
are acquiring total language knowledge at the same rate."
Visit www.babyonline.co.za for more information on pregnancy, birth and raising your baby.
Visit www.babyonline.co.za for more information on pregnancy, birth and raising your baby.